Beyond understanding basic math facts, children’s mathematical knowledge also involves knowing the words used to describe specific mathematical concepts and operations. There are two main types of mathematical language: quantitative words, and words related to spatial and ordinal relations. Research shows that building children’s understanding of these types of mathematical language supports their later mathematical learning[1]. This research review describes the two main types of mathematical language and introduces a range of games and activities that can be used to help children develop their understanding and use of mathematical language.
The first type of mathematical language involves quantitative words, and includes terms such as more, fewer, fewest, least, a lot, a little bit, add, give, combine, take away, same (number of physical objects or pictures), similar, and different. These words are critical to understand if children are to more generally (or approximately) describe the size of a set of objects (a lot, a little bit), compare groups of objects to determine the larger (more), smaller (fewer, fewest, least), or comparable (same, similar) set, and describe the process of changing the set size of quantities of objects (add, give, combine, take away). Studies show that children who have better knowledge of quantitative words in early childhood tend to also have better numeracy skills, including rote counting, comparing quantities, and adding simple sums[2]. This is thought to be because children acquire number skills better when they understand the mathematical language behind them. For example, when children are taught the meaning of words to compare quantities such as more, less, many and fewer, they are better able to compare and talk about the size of numbers and groups of objects. Also, when children do not know words to describe quantities such as ‘all,’ ‘most,’ and ‘some,’ they struggle to link number words to their respective sets of objects (cardinality). A recent study showed that when preschool-aged children read books emphasising quantitative words (particularly books from the Little Elephant Series) at home with their parents, their numeracy skills benefited compared to children exposed to similar books that did not emphasise quantitative words[3].
There are many opportunities for children to learn and use quantitative words in free and guided play. For example, using toy balance scales may allow children to apply math language words like ‘more, less, and the same’ to describe the relative magnitudes of different or the same quantities visually represented using the scale (for example, using the word ‘more’ when the left side of the scale is positioned lower than the right side, fewer when it is higher than the right side, or same when the two sides are level with each other). Teachers can also use quantitative language with children while they play with balance scales to describe how the positioning of the scale represents which quantity is fewer or greater. Teachers may also try to encourage children to create situations in which the left or right sides of the scale have more, fewer, or the same objects.Using playing cards can also allow children to use these same words to compare the quantities of pictured objects on two cards placed side-by-side (for example, ‘my 5-card has more than your 3-card’). Helping children play the card game War by suggesting specific quantitative words to use to generally describe and compare the amounts of objects on each of two cards can help children learn what these words mean.
When children are playing ‘shops’, they have the opportunity to use words that describe the process of changing the set size of quantities of objects (such as add, give, take away) in response to exchanging play money for store items such as play food (for example, ‘give me $5’; ‘let me add up the total for all of your groceries on my cash register’). In the block play area, when children are playing with a large amount of construction materials, they have the opportunity to use approximate quantitative magnitude language to more generally describe the size of the quantities they have (such as ‘a lot of red Lego blocks’ or ‘a little bit of yellow Lego’)[4]. Teachers can support this use of language by commenting on the relative amounts of toys, money, or blocks individual children have in relation to others, or using spatial language to describe how food and money are added to, taken away, or given when children are playing shops. Teachers can also show children different quantities of objects and help them use quantitative language to describe the quantities more generally. A full pot of paint could be described as a lot, for example, and a small amount of paint in a cup could be considered a little bit. While children have general concepts of quantities, teachers can play the critical role of introducing vocabulary to help give those concepts specific names.
Activities mentioned in these guides on Non-symbolic relations and combinations, Symbolic number skills, Connecting non-symbolic to symbolic representations, and Symbolic relations and combinations provide more opportunities for children to use quantitative mathematical language in free and guided play.
The second type of math language involves words related to spatial and ordinal relations. These words include:
- spatial location and relational terms such as nearest, closest, higher, lower, on top, below, above, bottom, far away, away, between, middle, inside, under, behind, and in
- ordinal terms such as first, last, middle, back, front, beginning, end, after, and before
- spatial features and properties terms such as edge, side, angle, corner, round, curved, flat, and wide
- shape terms such as circle, triangle, rectangle, rhombus or diamond, oval, star, heart, trapezoid, pentagon, hexagon, and octagon
- dimension terms such as big, large, long, wide, small, and tall
- spatial orientation terms such as upside down, diagonal, rotated, and flipped.
Knowledge of these words is important for understanding the relations between objects in space. Research studies show when children hear spatial words in infancy and toddlerhood, they develop better spatial skills as preschoolers[5]. Studies show that teaching preschool children spatial vocabulary during playful activities with experimenters involving block, shape, and Lego play (such as this magnetic Geoform shape building activity and this Lego set), origami (such as this whale shape and pig face), and shape matching, composing, matching shape halves, and shape sorting games (such as this set of shape sorting activities) actually leads to improvements in their shape knowledge and spatial thinking. There was less improvement when children played these activities with experimenters without hearing specific spatial language (for example, the experimenter uses only vague shape labels such as shapes and vague spatial terms like here and there)[6].
Construction[1] materials such as blocks and puzzles often include varied spatial forms that children can use to explore and compare spatial properties, such as noting their different edges (round or straight) and corners; observing whether the shapes have names based on these properties (like square or triangle); noting their sizes or dimensions (big, small, tall, wide), and how they are positioned or moved around in space (for example, upside down or flipped). Also, having multiple construction materials (such as in construction sets) to play with together allows children to compare and contrast the spatial properties of the same and different pieces within those sets, highlighting the different spatial properties of each. This is especially the case when using construction materials during building, since this involves combining different pieces to create different structures. For example, when building a vertical tower, a child might discover that the edges of a rectangle/rectangular prism block is longer than the edge of a square/cube when placed on top of each other. Lego sets in particular allow children to concretely see the number of pips that protrude after placing a smaller Lego block (like a cube shaped block) on top of a larger block, such as a rectangular prism. Magnetic building materials like Magformers, Geoform, and Magnatile blocks have multiple shapes of different sizes represented (including isoceles triangles, small and large equilateral triangles, squares, and other shapes in expansion packs like pentagons and rhombuses). They use magnetic force to encourage children to join individual block shapes along their edges, highlighting the spatial properties they have in common (such as their shared edge lengths).
Teachers can encourage children to learn and use spatial language by engaging them in spatially relevant guided play activities. Research shows that when adults such as parents play with puzzles and blocks with their children, they are naturally encouraged to use more spatial language than during activities that do not involve construction materials, especially when they work to build a specific model structure[7]. This is likely because working in pairs or groups on highly spatial tasks like building requires communication about spatial concepts to complete specific spatial goals. For example, during block building, pairs may want to communicate the types of blocks they are using (‘this is a square/cube’), where in relation to other blocks they are placing individual blocks (‘I’m placing this block on top of this one,’), and where they are planning to build components of the model structure (‘I’m building the door to my house and it goes in the middle’).
When completing a puzzle, adults might guide children about how to look at and describe individual pieces (for example, ‘this is an edge piece because it has a flat side’) and how best to turn individual pieces to fit into their designated spots (for example, ‘rotate your piece this way so the picture faces up, and so it fits here’). Adults and children can also discuss how the puzzle they are working on resembles the model image pictured on the box (for example, ‘this piece is part of the tree, and the tree is at the bottom of our puzzle’). For younger children, adults can use shape sorter sets to both label the shape names of each piece (‘this is a triangle’), discuss the locations and characteristics of the specific spaces into which each shape should be placed, and use language to describe how they or the children must rotate or flip the shape blocks to fit the space available.
Folding paper in specific ways, such as during origami tasks, requires the same types of talk about spatial concepts. For example, adults and children can describe how they are moving the paper in space (for example, flipping the paper over so that the coloured side is facing up), commenting on which aspect of the paper must be folded (for example, ‘the top of the square, goes down here’). Spatial ordinal language may also be naturally elicited when playing with string beading sets as children may practice ordering individual beads (first, last, in the middle) in their chosen sequence.
Activities mentioned in these guides on Repeating pattern skills and Spatial skills provide more opportunities for children to use spatial and ordinal mathematical language in free and guided play.
Math language type | Vocabulary | Definition/use | Example activity |
Quantitative | more, fewer, fewest, least, a lot, a little bit, add, give, combine, take away, same (number of physical objects or pictures), similar, and different | These words are used to: generally or approximately describe the size of a set of objects (a lot, a little bit)[2] compare groups of objects to determine the larger (more), smaller (fewer, fewest, least), or comparable (same, similar) setdescribe the process of changing the set size of quantities of objects (i.e., add, give, combine, take away) | Playing shops when exchanging items for money (e.g., ‘give me $5’) Balance scale play to describe whether the quantities are the same, or one side has more or fewer Playing cards (e.g., War) to compare whether one card’s quantities are the same, more, or fewer than another card’s quantities |
Spatial and Ordinal | |||
Spatial location and relational | nearest, closest, higher, lower, on top, below, above, bottom, far away, away, between, middle, inside, under, behind, and in | Used to describe the spatial location of objects in relation to other objects | Puzzle play Block play (Tangrams, wooden blocks, Magformers) |
Spatial features and properties | edge, side, angle, corner, round, curved, flat, and wide | Used to describe individual characteristics/parts of spatial forms | |
Shape | circle, triangle, rectangle, rhombus or diamond, oval, star, heart, trapezoid, pentagon, hexagon, and octagon | Used for names of spatial forms | |
Dimension | big, large, long, wide, small, and tall | Used to describe the size of spatial forms | |
Spatial orientation | upside down, diagonal, rotated, and flipped | Used to describe the spatial positioning of objects | |
Ordinal | first, last, middle, back, front, beginning, end, after, and before | Used to describe the positioning of objects, often in a linear arrangement or series | Bead stringing activities to describe the order in which beads are placed in a sequence (e.g., first/beginning, end, after, before) |
Endnotes
[1] Purpura, D. J., & Reid, E. E. (2016). Mathematics and language: Individual and group differences in mathematical language skills in young children. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 36, 259–268. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2015.12.020 ;
Purpura, D. J., Napoli, A. R., Wehrspann, E. A., & Gold, Z. S. (2017). Causal connections between mathematical language and mathematical knowledge: A dialogic reading intervention. Journal of Research on Educational Effectiveness, 10, 116–137. https://doi.org/10.1080/19345747.2016.1204639
[2] Hornburg, C. B., Schmitt, S. A., & Purpura, D. J. (2018). Relations between preschoolers’ mathematical language understanding and specific numeracy skills. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 176, 84–100. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2018.07.005
[3] Purpura, D. J., Schmitt, S. A., Napoli, A. R., Dobbs-Oates, J., King, Y. A., Hornburg, C. B., Westerberg, L., Borriello, G. A., Bryant, L. M., Anaya, L. Y., Kung, M., Litkowski, E., Lin, J., & Rolan, E. (2021). Engaging caregivers and children in picture books: A family-implemented mathematical language intervention. Journal of Educational Psychology, 113(7), 1338–1353. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000662
[4] Ramani, G. B., Zippert, E., Schweitzer, S., & Pan, S. (2014). Preschool children’s joint block building during a guided play activity. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 35(4), 326–336. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2014.05.005;
Zippert, E., Eason, S. H., Marshall, S., & Ramani, G. B. (2019). Preschool children’s math exploration during play with peers. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 65, 101072. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2019.101072
[5] Pruden, S. M., Levine, S. C., & Huttenlocher, J. (2011). Children’s spatial thinking: Does talk about the spatial world matter? Developmental Science, 14, 1417–1430. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01088.x
[6] Casasola, M., Wei, W. S., Suh, D. D., Donskoy, P., & Ransom, A. (2020). Children’s exposure to spatial language promotes their spatial thinking. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 149(6), 1116–1136. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000699;
Ferrara, K., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Newcombe, N. S., Golinkoff, R. M., & Lam, W. S. (2011). Block talk: Spatial language during block play. Mind, Brain, and Education, 5, 143–151. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-228X.2011.01122.x
[7] Zippert, E., Douglas, A.-A., Smith, M. R., & Rittle-Johnson, B. (2020). Preschoolers’ broad mathematics experiences with parents during play. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 192, 104757. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2019.104757